JOURNAL OF THE
ROYAL MEDICAL SERVICES

Official Publication for the Jordanian Royal Medical Services


Linguistic Factors Affecting the Loci and Frequency of Stuttering Across Age Groups among Arabic-Speaking Jordanians


Abdelrahim Attieh, Ph.D., SLP


Abstract

Objectives: This study was designed to study the association of stuttering loci and frequency with certain linguistic factors among stuttering Jordanian children and adults.

Method: Seventy four Jordanian persons who stutter, categorized into three age groups, constituted the subjects of the present study. Speech samples were collected from each subject’s oral reading and conversational speech, and analyzed according to the frequency of stuttering on each of the following linguistic factors: Word length, word grammatical class, word position, sentence position, and syntactical complexity. Paired t-tests and ANOVA were conducted to determine the significance of each linguistic factor.

Results: There were significant correlations between the stuttering loci and frequency with each of the linguistic factors studied.

Conclusion: The results are in accordance with findings in other languages. It was found that for our subjects, as it was the case for English, Norwegian, Kannada, German, and Spanish subjects, the stuttering events were strongly associated with word length, word grammatical class, word position, sentence position, and syntactical complexity.

Key words: Arabic, Jordanian, Linguistic factors, Loci of stuttering events, Stutter frequency

JRMS September 2010; 17(3): 10-19

 

Introduction

Stuttering is defined as a disturbance in the normal fluency and time pattern of speech that is distinguished by blocks, sound and syllable repetitions, and sound prolongations; and various types of normal disfluencies, such as interjections, broken words, whole-word repetitions, etc.; and might be accompanied by physical concomitants, such as eye-blinks, head jerks, limb movement, etc.(1)

Past research has shown that linguistic variables affect stuttering frequency and distribution in English-speaking persons who stutter (PWS). This relationship might explain the nonrandom distribution of stuttering events in the speech of disfluent speakers. For example, PWS were found to stutter more on longer words,(2-8) content rather than function words,(4,9-14) and syntactically complex utterances.(15-18) They also stutter on initial sounds of the words(19,20) and at the beginning of an utterance.(11,13,19,21-27) It should be noted that the word’s grammatical class influence on the stuttering of young children is opposite to that reported for older children and adult PWS. Children were consistently found to experience more difficulty on function words, whether using simple analysis(28) or analyzing the phonological words.(29-31) In addition, some studies(32-37) did not find significant differences between variations in syntactic complexity and stuttering frequency. Actually, syntactic difficulty might be an age-related factor.(17,35) Logan(38) cited many studies that related increased stuttering occurrences with syntactic complexity in young children. Their adolescent and adult participants, however, showed no difference in disfluency rates across the four levels of syntactic complexity they studied. Also, it is worthy to note that, whereas linguistic parameters may influence stuttering, it is not at all clear that deficits in language have a role in the development of stuttering.  In fact, several studies report that children at the onset of stuttering are not deviant in language skill.(39)

The association between certain linguistic factors and stuttering loci and frequency was denoted by some researchers to increased demands placed by those linguistic properties on speech motor planning and execution.(40-42) Three theories(43-45) proposed language involvement in stuttering.  Perkins et al.,(43) Postma & Kolk,(44) and Wingate(45) claimed that the disruption in fluency is due to slow lexicalization, i.e. the process by which speakers retrieve and encode words to maintain ongoing speech. Recently, Prins et al.(46) found that slow lexical processing by the stutterers could serve to disrupt fluency. Finally, the interaction between language  formulation  and  speech  motor  execution  had  been  adopted  by  many  researchers as an acceptable explanation for fluency breakdown.(41,47-49)

Language factors, in fact, overlap and interfere with each other during their influence on speech fluency. Designing an experiment to study a single factor without the interference of other factors presents several methodological problems. For example, in a study of the word length factor and frequency of stuttering, it is necessary to isolate factors such as word frequency, word information load, syllabic stress, and word position in an utterance. This, however, has never been completely controlled. Language factors do interfere and interact during running speech. Actually, a design aimed at isolating a given language factor results in artificial speech samples that are not representative of the stutterer's actual use during various speaking situations. A practical strategy to resolve this problem might be accomplished by securing sizable samples of the speaker's daily conversational speech and readings.

The study of language factors in stuttering is important both in terms of theory construction and clinical applications.(50) This point of view is strengthened by information about the significant role that language deficits might play in developing and maintaining components of stuttering.(51)  The universality of some or all language factors in defining the loci of stuttering events among various languages might highlight linguistic processing or speech execution deficits. Language factors in stuttering were investigated in a few languages other than English, such as Norwegian,(52) German,(53,54) Spanish(55) and Kannada(56-57) and were found to influence the stuttering of their speakers. Additional accumulated information from various languages will enrich our data-based knowledge, making inferences about language processing and execution, and their connection with stuttering, more universal. Bernstein Ratner and Benitez(58) emphasized the need for cross-linguistic research in this domain. To the author's knowledge, no pertinent previous study addressed the relationships between linguistic factors and stuttering in Arabic-speaking PWS.

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the impact of certain linguistic factors, such as word length, word type and grammatical class, word and sentence positions, and syntactical complexity upon the frequency of stuttering in three age groups of Jordanian persons who stutter, using comprehensive oral readings and conversational speech samples. In addition, the distribution of stuttering moments along words and short sentences was also surveyed.

 

Method

Subjects

Seventy-four Jordanian PWS were chosen randomly from a population of 132 stuttering cases referred to the Speech Clinic, Royal Rehabilitation Center, during a period of 17 months.  They were divided into three age groups. Group I included children from the first three grades (N=25, 20 males and 5 females, age range= 6.0–8.9 years; M=7.5 years; SD=±0.92). Group II included students in grades four to nine (N=23, 21 males and 2 females, age range=9.0–15.6 years; M=11.10 years; SD=±1.97). Group III included senior students and adults (N=26, 21 males and 5 females, age range 16.10–28.0 years; M=20.7 years; SD=±4.05).

Females were represented in the total sample according to their proportion in our clinic’s stuttering caseload which is approximately one in five. Each group contained various stuttering severity levels except the profoundly severe. No sub-classification according to severity was done due to the relatively small size of each age group.

 

Materials

Three   types  of   materials, oral  reading,  pictorial stimuli for spontaneous speech, and structured conversational questions, were prepared to be analogous of the daily speech of a person who stutters. The oral readings (written prose) included three word lists, a sentence list, and two 100 word passages, a syntactically complex passage and a syntactically simple passage. Different written materials were prepared for each of the two older groups commensurate with their expected reading skills. The youngest children did not do the reading task. The spoken prose consisted of the subjects’ responses to standard questions and conversational speech, and of naming, describing, and interpreting pictures. Care was taken to obtain a relatively comparable speech sample size from each subject. The total corpus collected from each prose type for each age group was used for analysis. A full description of the materials used to collect both prose types is presented in detail in Attieh.(59)

  The validity of the materials prepared for the present study was measured by using disciplined subjectivity.(60) Each component of these materials used was assessed by a three-member jury. The jury, experts in linguistics, rated their opinions regarding two issues, the appropriateness and the representativeness, according to a four-point scale: extremely significant (3), significant (2), fairly significant (1), and insignificant (0). A mean score was then calculated for each jury’s rating. Three items received the highest possible mean rating of 3 (extremely significant) and five items received mean ratings between 2 and 2.67.

 

Procedure

Each subject was first showed the set of pictures, one at a time, and asked to either name, describe, interpret, or answer a question, according to the card presented to him/her. The subject was then engaged in a comfortable structured conversation. Subjects of groups II and III were then asked to read the prepared word list, sentence list, and the two passages. The subjects’ responses were tape-recorded in a quiet room using Grunding (STENORETTE 2060) tape recorder.

Replays of the tape-recorded utterances of each subject of the study were transliterated. The transcripts were used to register instances of stuttering during another replay of the tape-recorded utterances. Only those behaviors that are considered as core behaviors61 of stuttering were registered. Another speech pathologist evaluated 19 (25.7%) of the tape-recorded material selected randomly and marked the instances of core stuttering. The inter-rater reliability(62) was 0.93 for the transliteration and 0.91 for the identification of stutter occurrences.

 

 Analysis

The first analysis performed was to relate the frequency of stuttering and the word length. Various word lengths were monosyllabic, bisyllabic, trisyllabic, and four or more syllabic words. The percentage of stuttering on each word length was individually calculated for each prose sample collected from each subject, and then was averaged over the subjects of each group. The prose is the speech samples from the reading, pictures, and conversational tasks. The second analysis was done by assigning a grammatical class for each word, spoken or orally read, as either content or function word. This was followed by calculating the percentage of stuttering on each content word and function word collected from each prose provided by each subject.

The third analysis addressed word and sentence position. The corpora were analyzed to count the number and length of utterances for each subject. Two analyses were done. First, the frequencies of stuttering occurrences on initial versus other word positions was calculated in both written prose and spoken prose. Secondly, the distribution of stutters along the words of two and three word sentences was analyzed. The results then were displayed in percentages and graphs.

The last analysis addressed comparing the relative number of stuttering events on oral readings of complex and simple passages (see Material above) by the older two groups. Paired t-tests for dependent variables were conducted to determine the significance of the difference in mean frequency of stuttering for all linguistic factors under analyses (except for the factor of word and sentence position, i.e., the thirds factor) for both the spoken prose and the written prose of the three age groups. A priori alpha level of these tests was set at 0.05.  Following the paired t-test analyses, analyses of variance were done to analyze each of the dependent variables with the age group and prose type.

 

Results

Word length factor

As shown in Table I, there was a gradual increase in the mean percentages of stuttering along with the increase in word length for all age groups regardless of prose type.

 

Table I. Mean percentage of stuttering on all four word lengths, along both prose types of the three age groups.

Age Group

Stimulus

Mono-syllabic Words

(Mean & SD)

Bisyllabic

Words

(Mean & SD)

Tri-syllabic

Words

(Mean & SD)

Longer Words

(Mean & SD)

Group I

Spoken Prose

23.4%

(9.0)

32.2%

(11.5)

36.7%

(15.2)

40.4%

(18.7)

Group II

Oral reading Prose

19.2%

(9.3)

22.6%

(11.7)

27.6%

(13.0)

29.9%

(14.0)

Spoken Prose

23.0%

(9.1)

25.9%

(11.0)

29.8%

(13.3)

37.3%

(17.4)

Group III

Oral reading Prose

15.1%

(8.3)

20.8%

(9.9)

26.0%

(11.6)

30.3%

(14.8)

Spoken Prose

14.4%

(7.1)

22.0%

(9.9)

28.0%

(11.8)

35.7%

(16.9)

 

Table II. Analysis of variance between the factor of word length and the factor of age

Word Length

Group

Mean

S. D.

Between- Groups F

Between- Groups P

Mono-syllabic Words

I

23.4

9.0

8.9

0.000*

II

21.2

9.8

III

14.7

9.0

Bisyllabic Words

I

32.2

11.5

7.5

0.001*

II

24.3

11.8

III

21.4

10.7

Tri-syllabic Words

I

36.7

15.2

4.7

0.011*

II

28.7

13.0

III

27.0

11.6

4 or more syllabic Words

I

40.4

18.7

1.5

0.219

II

33.7

19.0

III

33.1

16.0

 

Table III. Analysis of variance between the factor of word length and the factor of prose type

Word length

Prose

Mean

S. D.

Between- Groups F

Between- Groups

Significance

Mono-syllabic Words

Written

17.1

10.6

2.8

0.095

Spoken

20.2

9.3

Bisyllabic Words

Written

21.7

12.0

5.2

0.025*

Spoken

26.8

11.5

Tri-syllabic Words

Written

26.8

12.1

3.7

0.058

Spoken

31.6

13.9

4 or more syllabic Words

Written

30.1

15.7

5.3

0.024*

Spoken

37.9

18.5



This increase in stuttering from one word length to the next was statistically significant, with the exception of the difference between monosyllabic and bisyllabic words of both orally read and spoken prose of group II and between trisyllabic and longer words orally read by group II and the spoken prose of group I. The six pairs of various possible comparisons between different word lengths are significant at p≤0.001 when totaling the word length for each prose of all three age groups.

When the frequency of stuttering on each word length was analyzed according to age group, it can be noted in Table I that there was a consistent increase in the mean stuttering percentage from older age to younger age for all four word- lengths. As shown in Table II, analysis of variance (ANOVA) of the mean percentage of stuttering on the first three word-lengths were significant (p=0.000, 0.001, 0.011 respectively) along the three age groups, while the mean percentage of stuttering on four or more syllabic- words did not vary significantly between age groups. Also, as noted in Table III, there was a consistent increase in the mean percentage of stuttering between ‘written prose’ and ‘spoken prose’ for all four word-lengths. However, only the ‘between groups’ differences for di-syllabic and four or more syllabic-words had significant differences (p=0.025 and 0.024 respectively).

 

Table IV. Paired T-test between the mean percentage of stuttering on content and function words, according to prose type and age group

Age Group

Stimulus

Mean %  (SD) of Stutters on Content Words

Mean % (SD)   of Stutters on Function Words

T

df

Significance

(2-tailed)

Group 3

Written Prose

26.8%

(12.2)

13.8%

(7.0)

6.8

23

0.000*

Spoken Prose

25.6%

(11.8)

13.5%

(6.8)

5.0

25

0.000*

Group 2

Written Prose

25.8%

(13.0)

20.3%

(10.4)

3.0

20

0.007*

Spoken Prose

28.4%

(12.6)

16.3%

(7.3)

5.1

22

0.000*

Group 1

Spoken Prose

33.0%

(11.9)

26.3%

(9.4)

2.6

24

0.015*

All Groups

Both

28.0%

(12.4)

18.0%

(9.2)

9.6

118

0.000*

 

Table V. Analysis of variance for the interaction between the two prose types of the word class factor

Word- Grammatic Class

Prose Type

Mean of Stuttering

Standard Deviation

Between- Groups

F

Between- Groups

Significance

Content Words

Written

26.3

12.4

1.3

0.253

Spoken

29.0

12.3

Function Words

Written

16.8

8.3

0.9

0.329

Spoken

18.7

9.2

 

Table VI. Analysis of Variance for the interaction between various age groups of the word class factor

Word- Grammatic Class

Age

Group

Mean of Stuttering

Standard Deviation

Between- Groups

F

Between- Groups

Significance

Content Words

1

33.0

11.9

2.7

0.069

2

27.2

12.7

3

26.2

11.9

Function Words

1

26.3

9.4

16.2

0.000*

2

18.2

8.9

3

13.6

6.8

 

 Word Grammatical Class

As shown in Table IV, there was consistently more stuttering on ‘content’ words than on ‘functional’ words. This difference was significant (p=0.001) for the whole corpus collected by the three groups (Table V) and for each age group separately, whether the speech sample collected was ‘written prose’ or ‘spoken prose’.

The ANOVA for the interaction between age and prose type (Table V) shows that there were no significant differences in percentages of stuttering on either grammatical class of words among the three groups in respect to type of speech prose. However, only the percentage of stuttering on ‘functional’ words that showed statistical significance in respect to age group (Table VI). In other words, there was no statistical difference in the mean percentage of stuttering on content words between the three groups of the study. On the contrary, the mean percentage of stuttering on function words is statistically different from one age group to another, with an inverse relationship between the relative percentage of stuttering and the age of the subject. Also, as shown in Table V, there was no effect of the prose type on grammatical factor. In other words, there was no statistical difference in the mean percentage of stuttering on either content words or function words between ‘written prose’ and ‘spoken prose’ for both groups 2 and 3.

 

 Word and Sentence Position:

 The Word Position Factor

As shown in Table VII, the distribution of the occurrence of stuttering in words is invariably syllable-initial position (99.3%), and 84.1% of stutters occur in word-initial positions. Only 0.7% of stutters occur in syllable-final positions, which are mostly sound prolongations that, usually, mark a stutter on the subsequent word. The same finding is true regardless of the age group and the prose type. It is apparent that there was consistently more stutter occurrences in word-initial positions in the ‘written prose’ as compared to the ‘spoken prose’ for both age groups; however, these differences failed to be significant (p= 0.40 & 0.42, for groups 3 and 2, respectively).

 

Table VII. The mean percentage of stuttering on each word position, according to age group and prose type

Age group

Material

Mean % of stuttering on initial sounds of first syllable

Mean % of stuttering on initial sounds of other syllable

Mean % of stuttering on syllable- final sounds

3

Written prose

87.2 %

12.8 %

0.0 %

Spoken prose

82.5 %

16.2 %

1.3 %

2

Written prose

83.5 %

15.9 %

0.6 %

Spoken prose

79.5 %

18.1 %

2.4 %

Both

Both

84.1 %

15.2 %

0.7 %

 

Table VIII. Syntactical complexity: paired t-test comparisons between the mean percentage of stuttering on both ‘simple’ and ‘complex’ passages, for both groups i & ii subjects

Age group

Syntax

Paired Differences

Mean

 

Standard Deviation

 

T

Significance

(2-tailed)

 

 

Group 1

Simple - Complex

-5.15

4.90

- 4.70

0.000*

Group 2

Simple - Complex

-6.29

5.40

-5.71

0.000*

Both groups

Simple - Complex

-5.77

5.15

-7.4

0.000*

 

Table IX. Syntactical complexity: analysis of variance for the mean percentage of stuttering on each of ‘simple’ and ‘complex’ passages, for both group 2 & 3 subjects

Syntax

Age

Group

Mean

S. D.

Between- Groups

F

Between- Groups

Significance

Simple

1

19.50

9.47

0.71

0.403

2

16.92

10.58

Complex

1

24.65

11.33

0.168

0.684

2

23.21

11.87

 

The Sentence Position Factor

Analysis of the distribution of stutters along the words of a sentence was limited in this study to two and three word utterances. The distribution of stutters along two-word sentences is shown in Fig. 1, where around two thirds of stuttering occurred on the first word of the two- word sentences. Because subjects of group I had no reading material and the material of group III contained no two-word sentences, then the only comparison to make between both prose types was for group II. It can be noted that the strength of the first word as being stuttered is relatively stronger during the use of two- word sentences in conversation than during oral reading of these sentences.

Fig. 2 shows the distribution of stutters along three- word sentences for all three groups, including ‘written prose’ of group II. The mean percentage of stuttering consistently decreases from the first word to the subsequent words. The mean percentage of stuttering ranged between 40% - 48% for the first word, 32% - 36% for the second word, and 19% - 28% for the third word. Analysis of the distribution of stutters on longer utterances was left for future studies.

 

Syntactic difficulty

As shown in Table VIII, the mean percentages of stuttering on oral readings of the syntactically ‘complex’ passages, for each of group I and group II subjects and for the subjects of both groups combined, were statistically higher (p=0.001) than the syntactically ‘simple’ passages. ANOVA (Table IX) did not show a ‘between- groups’ difference for either the simple or complex passages, indicating this factor is stable for the two ‘reading’ groups of the study.

 

Discussion

As it is the case with PWS from other languages and cultures, Jordanian PWS also face more difficulty on longer words, on content rather than function words, on initial positions of words and sentences, and on oral readings of syntactically complex passages. These findings are viewed as an additional support for previous findings reported for other languages. 

The increased difficulty faced by persons who stutter on longer words, regardless of the spoken language, can be explained according to increased 'demands' and 'capacities'.(63)

2010Sept_Attieh_1.png

Fig. 1. The distribution of stuttering on two-word sentences, along both prose types & age groups. G refers to age group, S refers to spoken prose, and R refers to written prose


2010Sept_Attieh_2.png

Fig. 2. Distribution of stuttering along three-word sentences, along prose type and age group. 1 represents group III spoken prose, 2 represents group II spoken prose, 3 represents group II written prose, and 4 represents group I spoken prose


 Longer words require increased pressure on motor execution, rather than on word retrieval. Also note that the current data were balanced for word finding difficulties as the study compared the difficulty on a given word length in all stimulating possibilities, i.e. naming a picture and reading a written word versus conversational  speech.

The clear word grammatical class factor, for the distinction content versus function, and syntactical complexity factor can also be explained within the same framework. Content words, such as nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs that carry the meaning, require increased higher language processing in contrast to the function words, such as pronouns, articles, prepositions, etc. that do not carry full lexical meaning but have a grammatical or functional role.(31)  The reported lack of a clear grammatical factor for young children who stutter21 could not be tested because the youngest subject in the present study was six years old. However, as was shown in Tables 5, the mean percentage of stuttering on function words significantly decreased from the youngest age group to the older groups (26.3%, 18.3%, and 13.6%, for groups I, II, & III, respectively). This finding is in concordance with the reported literature.(30) The frequency of stuttering on content words, on the other hand, did not significantly vary across the three age groups.

When compared to the inconclusive reports of the syntactical complexity for English- speaking PWS, the results of the present study support, at least for Jordanian PWS, the tendency of increased stuttering occurrences to be associated with increased transformational demands,(15-18,64) i.e. synchronizing the logical content with the phonetic structure of complex sentences. The participants, regardless of age, but with few individual differences, faced more difficulty during oral reading of material that is complex in theme, structure, number of embedded clauses, and the length of the constituent sentences. This finding supports the hypothesis of syntactic complexity as a strong language factor in stuttering.

The present study also showed that the utterance length of only one to three syllables long resulted in differences in stuttering among the three age groups. Stuttering on longer utterances, four or more syllables, words did not vary among the age groups. Also, as far as the grammatical class of words, and, to a lesser degree, the word length factor, data showed that the prose type, whether read or spoken, did not have a different effect on these linguistic factors (refer to Tables III & V). In other words, the linguistic factors affect the frequency of stuttering regardless whether the word was read aloud or spoken.

The greater difficulty encountered by the stutterers at the beginning of an utterance can be explained as either due to increased linguistic demand,(11,27,45) increased neuromotor demand,(34,57) or both.(36) Brown(19) provided a psychological explanation. He accounted for the evident influence of both sentence position and word stress, in terms of their prominence in the sequence of the utterance. Presumably the prominence triggers the stutterer’s effort to avoid stuttering at these points. The mean percentage of stuttering on the first word of both two-word and three-word sentences was slightly higher for the spoken prose than for the written prose, for both groups II and III, and on all sentence lengths. However, this observation needs further research by balancing for other factors such as word length, word familiarity, information load, etc.

The current results should be taken cautiously due to certain limitations that need to be solved in future replications on Jordanian PWS. For example, the sample size did not allow for further comparisons according to the severity of the fluency disorder. In addition, during analysis of word and sentence position factor, no attempt was made to include syllabic stress, word information load, etc. in the analysis. The same applies to the position of the function words in the phonological words. Also, the results of the present study would be more meaningful when compared to the same language factors in preschool stuttering children.

 

Conclusion

The present study provided an additional support from another language for the association between stuttering events and certain linguistic components. All three age groups of Jordanian individuals who stutter faced more difficulty on longer words, on content words than on function words, on initial sounds of the words, and on words that are closer to the beginning of an utterance, whether the utterance was read aloud or spoken. During oral readings, the older two age groups showed increased difficulty on syntactically complex passages.

 

Acknowledgments

Thanks are due to my colleague Zaidan Khamayseh, PhD, for his contribution regarding the inter-rater reliability and to Mrs. Hanan Al-Hasan, for her contribution in data collection. Last but not least, I would like to thank all the anonymous Jordanian persons who stutter for kindly participating in this study.

 

References

1.    Bloodstein O. A Handbook on Stuttering, 5th ed. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group, Inc. 1995. p. 1-2.

2.   Bloodstein O, Grossman M. Early stutterings: Some aspects of their form and distribution. J Speech Hear Res 1981; 24: 298-302.

3.    Brown SF, Moren A. The frequency of stuttering with relation to word length during oral reading. J Speech Dis 1942; 7: 153-59.

4.    Danzger M, Halpern H. Relation of stuttering to word abstraction, part of speech, word length, and word frequency. Percept Motor Skills 1973; 37: 959-1062.

5.    Griggs S, Still AW. An analysis of individual differences in word stuttered. J Speech Hea Res 1979; 22: 572-80.

6.      Hejna FF. Stuttering frequency in relation to word frequency usage. Asha 1963; 5: 781.

7.    Quarrington B. Stuttering as a function of the information value and sentence position of words. J Abnormal Psych 1965; 70: 221-24.

8.    Silverman FH, Williams DE. Loci of disfluencies in the speech nonstutterers during oral reading. J Speech Hear Res 1967; 10: 790-94.

9.    Brown SF. The influence of grammatical function on the incidence of stuttering. J Speech Dis 1937; 2: 207-15.

10. Griggs S, Still AW. An analysis of individual differences in word stuttered. J Speech Hear Res 1979; 22: 572-80.

11. Quarrington B, Conway J, Siegel N. An experimental study of some properties of stuttered words. J Speech Hear Res 1962; 5: 387-94.

12. Silverman FH, Williams DE. Loci of disfluencies in the speech nonstutterers during oral reading. J Speech Hear Res 1967 10, pp790-794.

13. Soderberg GA. Linguistic factors in stuttering. J Speech Hear Res 1967; 10: 801-10.

14. Wingate ME. The first three words. J  Speech Hear Res 1979; 22: 604-12.

15. Bernstein Ratner BM, Sih CC. Effects of gradual increase in sentence length and complexity on children’s dysfluency. J Speech Hear Dis 1987; 52: 278-87.

16. Gaines ND, Runyan CM, Meters SC. A comparison of young stutterers’ fluent versus stuttered utterances on measures of length and complexity. J Speech Hear Res 1991; 34: 37-42.

17. Kadi-Hanifi K, Howell P. Syntactic analysis of the spontaneous speech of normally fluent and stuttering children. J Fluency Dis 1992; 17: 151-70.

18. Logan KJ, LaSalle LR. Grammatical characteristics of children’s conversational utterances that contain disfluency clusters. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1999; 42: 80-91.

19. Brown SF. A further study of stuttering in relation to various speech sounds. Quarterly Journal of Speech 1938; 24: 390-97.

20. Weiner AE. Stuttering and syllable stress. J Fluency Dis 1984; 94(4): 301-305.

21. Bloodstein O, Gantwerk BF. Grammatical function in relation to stuttering in young children. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1967; 10: 786-89.

22. Bernstein NE.  Are there constraints on childhood stuttering? J Fluency Dis 1981; 6: 341-50.

23. Hannah EP, Gardner JG. A note on syntactic relationships in nonfluency. J Speech Hear Res 1968; 11: 853-60.

24. Hejna FF. A Study of the Loci of Stuttering in Spontaneous Speech. Doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University, Evanston, Il. 1955.

25. Milisen RL.  Anticipation of stuttering as related to the frequency, type and point of occurrence of overt stuttering. Doctoral dissertation, University of Iowa, Iowa City 1937.

26. Taylor IK. The properties of stuttered words. J Verbal Learn Verbal Beh 1966; 5: 112-18.

27. Wells BG.  Effect of sentence structure on stuttering. J Fluency Dis 1979; 4: 123-29.

28. Bloodstein O. The development of stuttering: I. Changes in nine basic features. J Speech Hear Dis 1960; 25: 219-37.

29. Au-Yeung J, Howell P, Pilgrim L. Phonological words and stuttering on function words. J Speech, Lang Hear Res 1998; 41(5): 1019-1030.

30. Howell P, Au-Yeung J, Sackin S. Exchange of stuttering from function words to content words with age. J Speech Lang Hear Res1999; 42: 345-54.

31. Selkirk E. On the Major Class Features and Syllable Theory. In: Aronoff M, Oehrle RT, editors.  Language Sound and Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press 1984. P. 132-174.

32. Klouda GV, Cooper WE. Syntactic clause boundaries, speech timing, and stuttering frequency in adult stutterers. Language and Speech 1987; 30(3): 263-75.

33. Logan KJ, Conture EG. Length, grammatical complexity, and rate differences in stuttered and fluent conversational utterances of children who stutter. J Fluency Dis 1995; 20: 35-61.

34. Ronson I. Word familiarity and stuttering: The relationship to sentence structure. J Speech Hear Res 1976; 19: 813-19.

35. Silverman SW, Ratner NB. Syntactic complexity, fluency, and accuracy of sentence imitation in adolescents. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1997; 40(1): 95-106.

36. Wall MJ, Starkweather CW, Cairns HS. Syntactic influences on stuttering in young child stutterers. J Fluency Dis 1981; 6: 283-98.

37. Yaruss JS. Utterance length, syntactic complexity, and stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1999; 42(2): 329-44.

38. Logan KJ. The effect of syntactic complexity upon the speech fluency of adolescents and adults who stutter. J Fluency Dis 2001; 26: 85-106.

39. Watkins R, Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering III: Initial status of expressive language abilities. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1999; 42:  1125-35.

40. Adams M. The Demands and Capacities Model I: Theoretical elaborations. J Fluency Dis 1990; 15: 135-41.

41. Watson BC, freeman FJ, Devous MD, et al. Linguistic performance and regional cerebral blood flow in persons who stutter. J Speech Hear res 1994; 37(6): 1221-8.

42. Starkweather CW.  Learning and its role in stuttering development. In: Curlee RF, Siegel GM, editors. Nature and treatment of stuttering: New directions, 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon 1997. p. 79-98.

43. Perkins WH, et al. A theory of neuropsycholinguistic function in stuttering. J Speech Hear Res 1991, 34: 734-52.

44. Postma A, Kolk H. Error monitoring in people who stutter: Evidence against auditory feedback defect theories. J Speech Hear Res 1992; 35(5): 1024-32.

45. Wingate ME. The Structure of Stuttering: A Psycholinguistic Analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag 1988. p. 59-129.

46. Prins D, Main V, Wampler S. Lexicalization in adults who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1997; 40(2): 373-84.

47. Kleinow J, Smith A. Influence of length and syntactic complexity on the speech motor stability of the fluent speech of adults who stutter. J Speech Lang hear Res 2000; 43(2): 548-59.

48. van Lieshout PH, Starkweather CW, Hulstijin W, et al. Effect of linguistic correlates of stuttering on EMG activity in nonstuttering speakers. J Speech Hear Res 1997; 38(2):360-72.

49. Rastatter MP, Stuart A, Kalinowski J. Quantitative electroencephalogram of posterior cortical areas of fluent and stuttering participants during  reading  with  normal  and  altered  auditory feedback. Percept Mot Skills 1998; 87(2): 623-33.

50. Peters HFM, Starkweather, CW. The interaction between speech motor coordination and language processes in the development of stuttering: Hypotheses and suggestions for research. J Fluen Dis 1990; 15:115-125.

51. Homzie MJ, Lindsay JS. Language and the young stutter: A new look at old theories and findings. Brain and Language 1984: 22: 232-52.

52. Preus A., Gullikstad L, Grotterod H, et al. En undersoklse over forekomst av stamming I en lest tekst. Norsk tidsskrift for logopedi 1970; 16: 11-18.

53. Dworzynski K, Howell P, Au-Yeung J, et al. Stuttering on function and content words across age groups of german speakers who stutter. J Multilingual Comm Dis 2004; 2(2): 81-101.

54. Rommel D. The influence of psycholinguistic variables on stuttering in childhood. In Bosshardt HG, Yarruss JS, & Peters HFM editors. Fluency Disorders: Theory, research, treatment, and self-help. Proceedings of the Third World Congress of Fluency Disorders. Nyborg, Denmark. Nijmegen: Nijmegen University Press, 2001pp 195-202.

55. Howell P, Au-Yeung J. Phonetic complexity and stuttering in Spanish. Clin Linguist Phon 2007; 21(2): 111–127.

56. Jayaram M. Grammatical factors in stuttering. J Indian Institute of Science 1981; 63: 141-47.

57. Jayaram M. Phonetic influences on stuttering in monolingual and bilingual stutterers.  J  Comm  Dis 1983; 16: 287-97.

58.  Jayaram M. Distribution of stuttering in sentences- Relationship to sentence length and clause position. J Speech Hear Res 1984; 27: 338-41.

59. Bernstein Ratner NB, Benitez M. Linguistic analysis of a bilingual stutterer. J Fluency Dis 1985; 10(19): 211-219.

60. Attieh A. Investigating language factors In Jordanian Stutterers. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation 2002. Al-Neilain University, Khartoum, Sudan.

61. Erickson F. What makes school ethnography “ethnographic”? Anthropology and Education Quarterly 1973; 9: 58-69.

62. Guitar B. Stuttering: An integrated approach to its nature and treatment, 3rd ed. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins 2006.

63. Neale JM, Liebert RM. Science and Behavior: An Introduction to Methods of Research 1980, 2nd Ed. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

64. Hall NE, Yashamita TS, Aram DM. Relationship between language and fluency in children with developmental language disorders. J Speech hear Res 1993; 36(3): 568-79.

65. Weiss AL, Zebrowski PM. Dysfluencies in the conversations of young children who stutter: Some answers about questions. J Speech Hear Res 1992; 35: 1230-38.

About
The Journal

The Journal of the Royal Medical Services (JRMS) is an open access journal and it is the official publication for the Royal Medical Services of the Jordanian Armed Forces... Read More

Subscribe to OUR
newsletter

To receive updates on new issues

JRMS Journal

Articles Archive

Archive

Previous Issues

Volume 25
April 2018

Volume 24
December 2017

Volume 24
August 2017

Volume 24
March 2017